The Best Way to Study (It's Not What You Think)
A few months ago, at dinner, I told the kids about a book I was working on by Benedict Carey, a science reporter at The New York Times. The book is called How We Learn, and it's about all the sneaky, counterintuitive ways we learn -- in other words, the ways we learn outside of the school environment. I told Phoebe about one section of the book that takes on -- and takes down -- the idea of the dedicated study environment. That one place you go to study every night, at a certain time, with no distractions. (That was me, in college.) In fact, I told her, studies show that something as simple as changing where you study can lead to a 20% improvement in retention. Why? Because doing so increases the number of environmental cues the brain attaches to each piece of information being studied -- each passage of writing, each problem set -- making it easier to call it up when the time comes. Different desks, different songs playing in the background (yes, music is good), different times of day: All evidence shows that variation deepens and strengthens memory and retention. One thing about Phoebe: the girl takes things to heart. Ever since that conversation, she has been our little homework vagabond. She's upstairs, doing math at her desk, with her headphones on. She's reclined on the couch, reading The Westing Game, with the Premier League droning on in the background. She's downstairs at the kitchen table, head down, working on some fractions. She's standing at the kitchen counter, poring over a French vocab list. Will it work? We'll report back. But Ben's book is full of these kinds of ideas -- how sleep factors into learning, why flunking tests can be good for you, why distraction is not always a bad thing. It's a message that lowers the blood pressure a bit, and it happens to be backed up by science. He was kind enough to answer a few questions about his research here. -- Andy
Q: So, how important is routine when it comes to learning? For example, is it important to have a dedicated study area at home? A dedicated time of day?
Ben Carey: Not at all. Most people do better over time by varying their study or practice locations. The more environments in which you rehearse, the sharper and more lasting the memory of that material becomes – and less strongly linked to one “comfort zone.” That is, knowledge becomes increasingly independent of surroundings the more changes you make – taking your laptop onto the porch, to a café, on the plane. The goal, after all, is to be able to perform well in any conditions. Changing locations is not the only way to take advantage of the so-called context effect on learning, however. Altering the time of day you study also helps, as does changing how you engage the material, by reading or discussing, typing or writing by hand, sitting or standing up, studying in silence or while listening to music: each counts as a different learning “environment” in which you store the material in a different way.
Q: Is there an optimal amount of time to study or practice?
BC: More important than how long you study is how you distribute the study time you have. Breaking up study time – dividing it into two or three sessions, instead of one – is far more effective than concentrating it. If you’ve allotted two hours total to mastering some Spanish vocabulary, for example, you’ll remember more if you do an hour today and an hour tomorrow, or – even better – an hour today and an hour two days from now. That split forces you to re-engage the material, dig up what you already know, and then re-store it – an active mental step that reliably improves memory. Three sessions is better still, as long as you’re giving yourself enough time to dive into the material or the skills each time.
Q: Is cramming a bad idea?
BC: Not always, no. Cramming works fine as a last resort, a way to ramp up for an exam if you’re behind and have no choice. The downside is that, after the test, you won’t remember much of what you “learned” – if you remember any at all. That reason is that the brain, ironically, can sharpen a memory only after some forgetting has occurred. In this way, memory is like a muscle: a little “breakdown” allows it to subsequently build greater strength. Cramming, by definition, prevents this from happening.
Q: How much does quizzing oneself, like with flashcards, help?
BC: A lot, actually. Self-testing is one of strongest study techniques there is. Old-fashioned flash cards work fine; so does a friend, colleague, or classmate putting you through the paces. The best self-quizzes do two things: they force you to choose the right answer from several possibilities; and they give you immediate feedback, right or wrong. Self-examination improves retention and comprehension far more than an equal amount of review time. It can take many forms, as well. Reciting a passage from memory, either in front of a colleague or the mirror, is a form of testing. So is explaining it to yourself while kicking a ball against the wall, or having lunch with a friend. As teachers often say, “You don’t fully understand a topic until you have to teach it.” Exactly right. (And there’s the concept of pre-testing, which I wrote about in the New York Times Magazine last weekend, and is worth talking to your school about!)
Q: There’s so much concern that smartphones and all manner of electronic gadgets are interfering with learning -- and even changing the way people think. Is this true, though? Is distraction always bad?
BC: Distraction is a hazard to learning if you’re doing something that requires continuous focus, like listening to a lecture. But a short break – say, ten minutes to scroll through your Instagram feed, respond to a few emails, check some scores – is actually the most effective technique scientists know of to help you solve a problem when you’re stuck. Distracting yourself from the task at hand allows the brain to let go of mistaken assumptions, re-examine the clues in a new way, and come back fresh. If you’re motivated to solve the problem – whether it’s a proof, an integral, or a paragraph you just can’t get right – your brain will continue to work on it during the break offline, subconsciously, without the (fixated, unproductive) guidance you’ve been giving it.
Q: Why do we sometimes fail tests when we feel like we’ve tried so damned hard to prepare?
BC: The illusion that you “knew” something well just because it seemed so self-evident at the time you studied it. This is what learning scientists call fluency, the presumption that because something is well known now, it will remain that way tomorrow, or a week from now. Fluency illusions form automatically and subconsciously. Beware “study aids” that can reinforce the illusion: highlighting or re-writing notes, working from a teacher’s outline, re-studying after you’ve just studied. These are mostly passive exercise, and they enrich learning… not at all. Making your memory work a little harder – by self-quizzing, for example, or spacing out study time – sharpens the imprint of what you know, and exposes fluency’s effects.
Q: How does sleep affect learning?
BC: We now know that sleep has several stages, each of which consolidates and filters information in a different way. For instance, studies show that “deep sleep,” which is concentrated in the first half of the night, is most valuable for retaining hard facts – names, dates, formulas, concepts. If you’re preparing for a test that’s heavy on retention (foreign vocabulary, names and dates, chemical structures), it’s better to hit the sack at your usual time, get that full dose of deep sleep, and roll out of bed early for a quick review. But the stages of sleep that help consolidate motor skills and creative thinking – whether in math, science, or writing – occur in the morning hours, before waking. If it’s a music recital or athletic competition you’re preparing for, or a test that demands creative thinking, you might consider staying up a little later than usual and sleeping in. Exhaustion is the brain’s way of telling us that it’s time to stop. There’s little percentage in continued bleary-eyed staring at the social studies workbook or sawing away at the cello. The time has come to hit the hay — and let the sleeping brain finish the job.
Q: There’s a phrase you use in the book – to maximize our learning potential, you urge us to “free the inner slacker.” You suggest we should be less rigid about our studying. Recognize the brain is learning in all these other, more expansive ways, ways that may not conform to what we’ve been lead to believe about proper study habits. Is this a learning strategy?
BC: If it means drinking beer in front of the TV, then no. But to the extent that it means appreciating learning as a restless, piecemeal, subconscious and somewhat sneaky process that occurs all the time – not just when you’re at school, or sitting in a desk, faced pressed into a book – then it’s the best strategy there is. And it’s the only one available that doesn’t require more time and effort on your part, that doesn’t increase the pressure to achieve.